Unravel the multifaceted world of bilingualism and its profound impact on the human mind. Challenging the long-held belief in monolingualism as the norm, this exploration delves into the cognitive, psychological, and social dimensions of navigating life with two languages. Journey through the landscape of bilingualism research, where early studies suggesting intellectual inferiority gave way to groundbreaking discoveries of enhanced cognitive flexibility, analytical prowess, and communicative sensitivity in bilingual individuals. Discover Cummins' influential hypotheses, which attribute these advantages to enriched experiences, the cognitive agility fostered by language switching, and the heightened linguistic awareness born from comparing and contrasting languages. However, the narrative cautions against oversimplification, acknowledging the complexities of bilingualism and the varying degrees of proficiency. Explore Cummins' Threshold Theory, which posits that a certain level of language competence is necessary to unlock the cognitive benefits of bilingualism and avoid potential drawbacks. Uncover the distinction between basic interpersonal communicative skills (BICS) and cognitive academic language proficiency (CALP), shedding light on the challenges faced by minority language children in educational settings. Delve into the crucial role of attitude and motivation, as illuminated by Lambert's concepts of additive and subtractive bilingualism, and Gardner's socio-educational model, which emphasizes the interconnectedness of social background, aptitude, motivation, and language acquisition context. This synthesis of research and theory provides a comprehensive understanding of the intricate factors that shape the bilingual experience, offering valuable insights for educators, parents, and anyone seeking to appreciate the richness and potential of multilingualism in an increasingly interconnected world. Ultimately, this exploration highlights the significant social impact of bilingualism research, paving the way for a more inclusive and equitable society that embraces linguistic diversity and fosters positive attitudes towards different cultures and languages. Explore the intricate interplay of language, cognition, and society, and discover how bilingualism can enrich not only the individual but also the world around them.
1. Introduction
although the average European or US American believes that monolingualism is natural and normal, most people of the world are bilingual or even multilingual. in many Asian and African countries, people speak their home language and an official administrative language in order to communicate with members of other ethnical groups within their country.
As bilingualism is a reality in our world, psychologists, linguists, sociologists, teachers and, of course, parents began to ask: 'What consequences does bilingualism bring about for the individual?' , 'Will children suffer if they become bilingual?', 'Which role do attitude and motivation play in becoming bilingual?' In may essay, I want to give a short overview of research findings and theories which offer probable answers to these questions.
2. Definitions and Problems
Bilingualism is not an all-or-none property, therefore it may exist in various degrees (Hornby 1977). As language consists of the four basic skills, such as listening, reading, speaking and writing which in their turn are subdivided into further skills like vocabulary, grammar, style, pronunciation and meanings (Baker 1988), a large number of varieties is possible in which bilinguals may differ from each other in their language competence. Taking into account also social background, different roles of the languages involved etc., it seems to be impossible to define a bilingual. Scientists have tried to evade this problem by studying mostly balanced bilinguals, that is individuals with nearly equal competence in both languages. This group, however, may not be considered representative as the majority of bilinguals tends to have one dominant language (Grosjean 1982).
A second problem arises when groups are to be compared and therefore matched in their degree of intelligence. Theories of intelligence measurement vary from 150 factors to be considered (Guildford, Baler 1988) up to the possibility of reducing
intelligence to one ground factor (the 'g' factor of Spaerman and Jones). None of these categorisations has remained without critics.
3. Findings on Intellectual Effects
Up to the 1960s, nearly all studies found that bilinguals are inferior in intelligence to monolinguals. Those results, however, may be neglected as 'little care was taken to check out the essentials before comparing monolingual and bilingual subjects“ (W. E. Lambert, 1977)1.
The start to the belief in positive effects of bilingualism was given when in 1962 Elizabeth Pearl and Wallace E. Lambert found that Canadian French-English bilinguals were superior to their monolingual peers in verbal and non-verbal measures of intelligence. Several follow-up studies seemed to confirm that result. Superiority has been found in divergent thinking (special type of cognitive flexibility), analytical approach towards language, communicative sensitivity and field independence (Grosjean 1982; Baker 1988).
J. Cummins (1976) introduced three hypotheses to explain and integrate these findings. He attributes the higher scores of bilinguals in intelligence tests to:
1 a wider and more varied range of experiences bilinguals have,
2 the switching mechanism - bilinguals have to switch between their languages
and are therefore more flexible in thought.; moreover, two languages offer two different perspectives which might again lead to more flexibility and originality, and
3 the process of objectivation - bilinguals have to compare and contrast
meanings, grammatical forms etc.; this leads to constant vigilance and inspection of their languages (Baker 1988).
Nevertheless, we have to be cautious in evaluating these results. Why somebody becomes bilingual whereas another remains monolingual whilst measurable essentials seem to be the same is not explained. Perhaps the positive findings are the cause rather than the result of bilingualism (Baker 1988).
4. Individual Psychological Explanation
Another point is that there is no unanimity concerning the degree of superiority bilinguals have, and even inferiority has been found with non-balanced bilinguals. Cummins therefore argues that the important issue is not what effects bilingualism has on cognitive processes but which conditions facilitate or retard cognitive growth (Baker 1988).
One attempt to answer this question is Cummins' Threshold Theory. He sets two thresholds of language competence which are necessary to avoid negative consequences on the one hand, and enable the bilingual to benefit from positive effects on the other. Although it is not possible to define the threshold levels in absolute terms, one can roughly state that when a child has age-appropriate language proficiency in one language, the first threshold is reached. Being equally competent in both languages, positive effects on intellectual capability may result (Grosjean 1982).
In connection with this, Cummins argues that conversational skills are not closely related to academic performance as they are context-embedded and require rather cognitively undemanding language use. In the classroom, however, a higher level of language competence is needed as language used is context-reduced and cognitively demanding. Thus Cummins distinguishes between basic interpersonal communicative skills (BICS) and cognitive academic language proficiency (CALP) (Romaine 1989).
This may help to understand why children of minority groups often have a rather limited school success and score lower in intelligence tests. They have gained basic communicative skills in their first language at home and some knowledge in a second language playing with peers of the majority group. But when attending school, they cannot properly understand the teacher and fail. They miss out important information and experiences and show detrimental effects in comparison to monolingual children.
Studies have shown that teaching children of minority groups in their first
language at primary school - and thereby enabling them to have age-appropriate language skills - results in positive effects in the second language too (studies of
Long & Padilla 1970, Peal & Lambert 1962, Tucker & Lambert 1972; in Hornby 1977).
Although Cummins' theories explain a variety of findings, empirical confirmation is still required. Furthermore, he does not take into account other influences, such as the relationship between social-environmental factors, tratis of personality, attitude and motivation which a number of other researchers have recognised as essential.
5. Attitude / Motivation Models
W. E. Lambert (1977), for instance, argues that the social setting may provoke positive or negative consequences for bilinguals. In his terms he speaks of additive bilingualism (enrichment of language competence) and subtractive bilingualism (impoverishment of language competence). He considers a social setting where no pressure to replace and reduce the first language is likely to bring about additive bilingualism, whereas circumstances requiring assimilation and replacement of the first language, enhancing the rear of identity loss, may lead to subtractive bilingualism. This might explain why school immersion programmes have only been successful with children of the majority group.
Together with R. C. Garnder, W. E. Lambert suggests another aspect which influences the success of becoming bilingual. In their studies both scientists found that there are two main motivations which lead to bilingualism. The instrumental motivation implies utilitarian motives: Lambert defines it as „the desire to gain social recognition or economic advantages through knowledge of a foreign language.“1 The integrative motivation means personalised or interpersonal motives: „the desire to be like representative members of the 'other language community'.“2 Depending on the context, these motivations may be fostered or inhibited. Both scientists realised, however, that according to their results the integrative motivation is the more influential (Baker, 1988).
One of the most inclusive models, however, seems to be R. C. Gardner's socio- educational model. He presents 4 influential stages which interact with each other and thus build a cyclical and dynamic model.
The first stage describes the social and cultural background. The child grows up in a certain community which transmits beliefs about culture and language. These factors may determine intelligence, language aptitude, motivation and situational anxiety which form stage 2. The third stage comprises the context in which language is acquired, whether it is rather formal or informal. Obviously, situational anxieties may prevent informal language learning, or the social background may only allow formal language acquisition. The fourth stage is the so-called output, e. g. bilingual proficiency on the one hand and attitudes, self-concept, cultural values and beliefs on the other. Stage four, in its turn, may have consequences for preliminary stages as bilingual proficiency probably decreases situational anxieties, or a positive attitude may change the motivation.
R. C. Garnder's social-educational model has been the most universally and empirically tested theory so far. It implies the integrative motivation as an important variable, it shows how personality traits influence attitude and motivation, how attitude causes motivation and how motivation through self-confidence is an indirect cause of achievement (Baker 1988).
6. Conclusion
The study of bilinguals is far from giving clear answers. The questions I quoted at the beginning of my essay can only be responded to by theories and hypotheses which still fail to give clear evidence and which have several weaknesses.
Nonetheless, I think that it can be stated that bilingualism does not necessarily bring about negative effects on the individual. Whether it leads to positive consequences, one cannot definitely say.
Therefore, several psychologists, linguists and sociologists focus their research on the problem which circumstances foster and which inhibit the intellectual development of a bilingual. I have given a brief overview of some important models and theories because I believe that the complexity of this subject can only be understood when having a variety of different approaches to it. Language
competence, social setting, motivation and attitude are only a few of the many aspects which influence bilingualism and which interact with each other.
The value of bilingualism research, however, consists in the great social impact it may have. The result and findings may help a numerous population to cope with their bilingual existence, to improve their status and self-image, to diminish societal anxieties and, altogether, to increase positive attitudes towards other national or ethnical groups.
Bibliography
Baker, Colin. Key Issues in Bilingualism and Bilingual Education. Clevedon and Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters Ltd 1988.
Grosjean; François. Life with Two Languages - An Introduction to Bilingualism. Cambridge (Mass.) and London: Harvard University Press 1982.
Hornby, Peter, A et al. Bilingualism - Psychological, Social and Educational
Implications. New York, San Francisco, London: Academic Press Inc. 1977.
Romaine, Suzanne. Bilingualism. New York: Basil Blackwell Inc. 1989.
[...]
1 Page 15 in Hornby ed al.
1 Page 153 in Baker 1988
Frequently Asked Questions About Bilingualism
What is the main topic discussed in this text?
The text provides an overview of bilingualism, exploring its definitions, the intellectual effects on individuals, psychological explanations, and the role of attitude and motivation in becoming bilingual.
How does the text define bilingualism?
The text defines bilingualism as not an all-or-none property but existing in various degrees, considering the four basic language skills (listening, reading, speaking, and writing) and their subdivisions. It also acknowledges the difficulty in defining a bilingual due to variations in language competence and social background.
What were the initial findings on the intellectual effects of bilingualism?
Initially, studies suggested that bilinguals were inferior in intelligence compared to monolinguals. However, these results were later questioned due to methodological issues in the studies.
What are some of the positive intellectual effects associated with bilingualism, according to later research?
Later research found that bilinguals can be superior in divergent thinking, analytical approach towards language, communicative sensitivity, and field independence.
What is Cummins' hypothesis regarding the intellectual advantages of bilinguals?
Cummins proposed three hypotheses: a wider range of experiences, the switching mechanism between languages leading to cognitive flexibility, and the process of objectivation involving comparison and contrast of languages.
What is Cummins' Threshold Theory?
Cummins' Threshold Theory states that there are two thresholds of language competence. Reaching the first threshold avoids negative consequences, and reaching the second threshold (equal competence in both languages) enables individuals to benefit from positive intellectual effects.
What are BICS and CALP, according to Cummins?
BICS (basic interpersonal communicative skills) refers to conversational skills, while CALP (cognitive academic language proficiency) refers to the higher level of language competence needed for academic performance in context-reduced and cognitively demanding situations.
How does the text explain the academic struggles of children from minority groups?
The text suggests that these children may have basic communicative skills in their first language but lack the necessary CALP in the language of instruction, leading to academic difficulties and lower scores in intelligence tests.
What is additive and subtractive bilingualism, according to W. E. Lambert?
Additive bilingualism refers to the enrichment of language competence, while subtractive bilingualism refers to the impoverishment of language competence. The social setting plays a significant role in determining which type of bilingualism occurs.
What are the two main motivations that lead to bilingualism, according to W. E. Lambert and R. C. Gardner?
The two main motivations are instrumental motivation (gaining social recognition or economic advantages) and integrative motivation (desire to be like members of the other language community). Integrative motivation is considered more influential.
What are the stages in R. C. Gardner's socio-educational model?
The stages are: (1) social and cultural background, (2) individual differences (intelligence, language aptitude, motivation, situational anxiety), (3) language acquisition context (formal or informal), and (4) output (bilingual proficiency, attitudes, self-concept, cultural values).
What is the conclusion of the text regarding the effects of bilingualism?
The text concludes that bilingualism does not necessarily lead to negative effects and may potentially lead to positive consequences. However, further research is needed to understand the circumstances that foster or inhibit intellectual development in bilinguals.
What are some of the aspects which influence bilingualism?
Some of the aspects which influence bilingualism are: Language competence, social setting, motivation, and attitude. All of these aspects interact with each other.
- Quote paper
- Sixta Quaßdorf (Author), 1991, Bilingualism - A Short Overview of Research on Intellectual Effects and the Conditions Which Influence Intellectual Development, Munich, GRIN Verlag, https://www.grin.com/document/105985